Does disaggregated electricity feedback reduce domestic electricity
consumption?A systematic review of the literature
Jack Kelly
jack.kelly@imperial.ac.uk
(Swipe or press right-arrow on your keyboard to change slides)
Background video
by Guryanov
Andrey / shutterstock
Evidence that NILM can help save energy...
1) People want disaggregated energy data
2) Behaviour affects energy consumption
modifying behaviour → reduce energy consumption
3) People are bad at estimatingthe energy consumption of their appliances
→ Fix the ‘information deficit’ then users can operate as rational ‘resource
managers’
(I’m now sceptical of this idea)
4) Multiple studies report that disaggregated
feedback reduces energy consumption
Systematic reviews
- Common in
medicine, social sciences etc.
- Distinct from ‘narrative’ reviews
- Aim to collect all papers matching a defined search criteria
- Quantitative summary of each paper and biases
- Quantitative synthesis of all results
Background image from UCSF
Literature search
Three search engines: Google Scholar,
the ACM Digital Library and IEEE Xplore
Search terms:
- ‘disaggregated AND
[energy|electricity] AND feedback’
- ‘N[I|A|IA]LM AND
feedback’
Searched papers’ bibliographies
Sent draft literature review to
authors for comments
The studies
12 groups of studies identified
Q1. Can disaggregated electricity feedback enable ‘energy enthusiasts’ to save energy?
- Very likely...
- Weighted-mean energy reduction = 4.5%
- A lot of uncertainty...
The Hawthorne Effect
- Hawthorne effect is illustrated by
Schwartz et al. 2013:
- Randomised controlled trial
- 6,350 participants split into 2
groups: control & treatment
- Treatment received weekly
postcard saying: ‘You have been selected to be
part of a one-month study of how much electricity you
use in your home... No action is needed on your
part. We will send you a weekly reminder postcard
about the study...’
- Treatment group reduced energy consumption by 2.7%!
- Failure to control for Hawthorne very likely to be
strong positive bias
- 8 studies did not control for Hawthorne
Other biases
- 6 studies used attention-grabbing
displays
- Home-visits
- 10 studies were short (4 months or
less)
- Cherry-picking statistical analyses
and comparison periods?
- 8 studies used sub-metered data,
hence avoiding mistrust from participants
- Publication bias?
Q2. How much energy would the whole population save?
- All 12 studies suffer from ‘opt-in’ bias
- Subjects self-selected
hence are probably more interested in energy than the average person
- Very likely to be a strong positive bias
Q3. Aggregate versus disaggregated feedback
- 4 of the 12 studies directly
compared disaggregated against aggregate feedback
- 3 studies found aggregate to
be more effective
- 1 study found aggregate to
be equally effective
- 2 field trials & 2 lab experiments
Sokoloski’s results
Energy reductions:
- IHD: 8.1% (statistically significant)
- Disaggregation: 0.5%
- Control: -2.5%
Sokoloski’s results
Findings from surveys:
- Follow-up survey revealed that the
disag group were not significantly more likely
to be willing to replace large, inefficient appliances
compared to controls or IHD group.
- Neither controls nor the disag group
significantly increased their perception of control
(initial survey versus follow-up).
- IHD group did increase
their perception of control.
Sokoloski’s results
Findings from surveys:
- Users viewed their devices:
- 0.86 times per day for disag users
- 8.16 times per day for IHD users
PG&E 2014 trial results
- IHD users significantly more likely
to report taking actions to reduce electricity usage
and to use their device to deduce power demand of
individual appliances(!)
- Several users did not
trust the disag data.
- IHD more successful in communicating
power demand now
Bidgely have redesigned their website since these studies
Conclusions
- NILM has many uses! This talk just considered one use!
- Available evidence suggests that
aggregate feedback is more effective than
disag feedback
- But these results
confounded by effect of IHD versus website
- Disag feedback might drive savings
of 0.7% - 4.5% in general population
- Disag feedback might drive larger savings
in ‘energy enthusiast’ populations
- Fine-grained disag may not be
necessary
- But! Lots of gaps in our knowledge.
Cannot robustly falsify any hypotheses yet.
Suggestions for future studies
- Compare aggregate versus disagg
(both on an IHD)
- Compare 2 groups:
- Aggregate on an IHD
- Aggregate (on an IHD) + disagg (on a website)
- Compare fine-grained disag versus
coarse-grained disag
- If you have data then please consider
releasing it; or writing a paper; or collaborating with
someone who will write a paper with you!
Users might become more interested in disag feedback if:
- Energy prices increase
- Concern about climate change
deepens
- Disag accuracy increases or if
designers communicate uncertain estimates
- Lots of ideas in the literature
about how to improve disag feedback. e.g. disag
by behaviour; or display feedback near
appliances; or provide better recommendations etc.
Does disaggregated electricity feedback reduce domestic electricity
consumption?A systematic review of the literature
Jack Kelly
jack.kelly@imperial.ac.uk
(Swipe or press right-arrow on your keyboard to change slides)
Background video
by Guryanov
Andrey / shutterstock