Do disaggregated electricity bills really help people to save energy?
Jack Kelly
jack.kelly@imperial.ac.uk
(Swipe or press right-arrow on your keyboard to change slides)
Outline
Introduction to energy disaggregation
Introduction to systematic reviews
Methodology
The studies
Findings
Gaps in our knowledge: Suggestions
for future research
Conclusions
The many names of 'energy disaggregation'
NILM: Non-Intrusive Load Monitoring
NALM: Non-intrusive Appliance Load Monitoring
NIALM: Non-Intrusive Appliance Load Monitoring
Bidgely raised $16.6 million in 2015
Why bother with disaggregation?
GB Smart Meter Roll-out
- All homes to have a smart
meter by 2020.
- These reports whole-house power demand
every 10 seconds to home area network (HAN).
- DECC's business case assumes that smart
meters will drive savings of £4.6 billion
due to reduced energy consumption (across both
electricity and gas).
Use-cases for energy disaggregation
-
Many use-cases
- This talk is about one use-case:
- Can disaggregated energy feedback help people to
reduce energy consumption more effectively than
aggregate energy data alone?
How might disaggregated data reduce energy demand?
"Information deficit" and "rational resource managers"
People self-report that they want disaggregated
energy data
But do people save energy when given
disaggregated data?
Why reduce energy consumption?
2015 Paris
agreement on Climate Change
"[Hold] the increase in the global average [surface] temperature
to well below 2 °C above pre-industrial levels and to pursue efforts
to limit the temperature increase to 1.5 °C above pre-industrial
levels"
United Nations Framework Convention on Climate
Change, COP
21,
Paris
Agreement, 2015-12-11
Background image from The Guardian/Francois Guillot/AFP/Getty Images
Background image from phys.org/Gregory Heath/CSIRO
Fossil-fuel emissions estimated to be compatible with 2
°C (RCP2.6)
Background image from phys.org/Gregory Heath/CSIRO
My Work
The Computer Science of disaggregation
Systematic reviews
- Common in
medicine, social sciences etc.
- Aim to
collect all papers matching a defined
search criteria
- Quantitative
summary of each paper and biases
- Quantitative
synthesis of all results
- May include a "meta-analysis"
- Distinct from
"narrative" reviews
Background image from UCSF
Research questions
Can disaggregated energy data help an
already-motivated sub-group of the general population
(‘energy enthusiasts’) to save energy?
How much energy would the general
population save if given disaggregated data?
Is fine-grained disaggregation
required?
For the general population, does
disaggregated energy feedback enable greater savings than
aggregate data?
Literature search
Three search engines: Google Scholar,
the ACM Digital Library and IEEE Xplore
Search terms:
- ‘disaggregated AND
[energy|electricity] AND feedback’
- ‘N[I|A|IA]LM AND
feedback’
Searched papers' bibliographies
Send draft literature review to
authors for comments
The studies
12 groups of studies identified
Findings
- Mean energy reduction = 4.5%
- Weighted by number of
participants
- Full meta-analysis probably not possible
Opt-in bias
- All 12 studies suffer from 'opt-in'
bias
- Subjects self-selected to some extent
- Subjects probably more interested in energy
than the average person
- Very likely to be a strong positive bias
The Hawthorne Effect
- 8 studies did not control for Hawthorne
- Hawthorne effect is illustrated by
Schwartz et al. 2013:
- Randomised controlled trial
- 6,350 participants split into 2
groups: control & treatment
- Treatment received weekly
postcard saying: "You have been selected to be
part of a one-month study of how much electricity you
use in your home... No action is needed on your
part. We will send you a weekly reminder postcard
about the study..."
- Treatment group reduced energy consumption by 2.7%!
- Failure to control for Hawthorne very likely to be
strong positive bias
Other biases
- 6 studies used attention-grabbing
displays
- Home-visits
- 10 studies were short (4 months or
less)
- Cherry-picking statistical analyses
and comparison periods?
- 8 studies used sub-metered data,
hence avoiding mistrust from participants
- Publication bias?
Can disaggregated electricity feedback enable "energy
enthusiasts" to save energy?
- Very likely. For example...
Home Energy Analytics (HEA) studies
- 1,623 users
- Up to 44 months
- Average reduction of 6.1%
- Top quartile (310
"super-enthusiasts") reduced by 14.5%
2014 PG&E study
- 1,685 users: half got IHD; half got Bidgely
- 3 months
- No significant reduction across all
1,685 users
- But users who selected a time-of-use
tariff saved 7.7% (142 IHD; 136 Bidgely)
How much energy would the whole population
save?
- No "perfect" correction for opt-in
bias
- Consider study in Sweden (Vassileva
et al. 2012):
- 2,000 households given access to
website analysing their aggregate energy demand
- Only 32% accessed the
website. They saved 15%.
- Those who did not access website
did not reduce energy.
- Average saving = 32% x 15% = 5%
How much energy would the whole population
save?
- Average opt-in rate = 16%
- Average saving across population =
16% x 4.5% = 0.7%
Is "fine-grained" disaggregation necessary?
Is "fine-grained" disaggregation necessary?
Home Energy Analytics (HEA) studies
- Average reduction of 6.1%
- Coarse-grained disaggregation may be
sufficient
- But no studies directly compared
fine-grained against coarse-grained.
Aggregate versus disaggregated feedback
- 4 of the 12 studies directly
compared disaggregated against aggregate feedback
- 3 studies found aggregate to
be more effective
- 1 study found aggregate to
be equally effective
- 2 field trials & 2 lab experiments
Sokoloski's results
Energy reductions:
- IHD: 8.1% (statistically significant)
- Disaggregation: 0.5%
- Control: -2.5%
Sokoloski's results
Findings from surveys:
- Follow-up survey revealed that the
disag group were not significantly more likely
to be willing to replace large, inefficient appliances
compared to controls or IHD group.
- Neither controls nor the disag group
significantly increased their perception of control
(initial survey versus follow-up).
- IHD group did increase
their perception of control.
Sokoloski's results
Findings from surveys:
- Users viewed their devices:
- 0.86 times per day for disag users
- 8.16 times per day for IHD users
- Returning devices:
- 2 of 7 (29%) wanted to return disag device
- 2 of 30 (7%) wanted to return IHD
PG&E 2014 trial
- 1,685 PG&E customers
- 1,001 from SmartRate & 584 from
time-of-use users
- additional no-contact controls
- Half got IHD & half got Bidgely
- 3 months
- Users choose intervention
- Did not tease apart consumption of
IHD vs Bidgely
- Churchwell et
al., HAN
Phase 3 Impact and Process Evaluation Report,
technical report by Nexant, 2014
PG&E 2014 trial results
- No significant energy reduction
across all 1,001 SmartRate users or 208 EV TOU users
- 7.7% energy reduction for time-of-use users
(142 IHD & 136 Bidgely users)
- IHD users significantly more likely
to report taking actions to reduce electricity usage
and to use their device to deduce power demand of
individual appliances(!)
PG&E 2014 trial results
- Most common complaint from Bidgely
users was about the disag feature. Several users did not
trust the disag data; or were unsure whether users should
assist the algorithm by turning loads on or off; or
thought categories were too few or too broad; or didn't
like that they couldn't add new disag categories.
- IHD more successful in communicating
power demand right now
PG&E 2014 trial results
Frequency of viewing devices
PG&E 2014 trial results
Percentage of customers saying they saved energy
PG&E 2014 trial results
Reported actions taken in response to feedback
Bidgely have redesigned their website since these studies
Suggestions for future studies
- Compare aggregate versus disagg
(both on an IHD)
- Compare 2 groups:
- Aggregate on an IHD
- Aggregate (on an IHD) + disagg (on a website)
- Compare fine-grained disag versus
coarse-grained disag
Conclusions
- There are many uses for
disag data! This talk just considered one use!
- Available evidence suggests that
aggregate feedback is at least as effective as
disag feedback
- Disag feedback might drive savings
of 0.7% - 4.5% in general population
- Disag feedback might drive larger savings
in "energy enthusiast" populations
- Fine-grained disag may not be
necessary
- But! Lots of gaps in our knowledge.
Cannot robustly falsify any hypotheses yet.
Conclusions
Users might become more interested in disag feedback if:
- Energy prices increase
- Concern about climate change
deepens
- Disag accuracy increases or if
designers communicate uncertain estimates
- Lots of ideas in the literature
about how to improve disag feedback. e.g. disag
by behaviour; or display feedback near
appliances; or provide better recommendations etc.
Do disaggregated electricity bills really help people to save energy?
Jack Kelly
jack.kelly@imperial.ac.uk
(Swipe or press right-arrow on your keyboard to change slides)